Regression done right: Part 3: Forecasts to believe in

There are three Sources of Uncertainty in a forecast.

  1. Whether the forecast is of “an environment that is sufficiently regular to be predictable”.1
  2. Uncertainty arising from the unexplained (residual) system variation.
  3. Technical statistical sampling error in the regression calculation.

Source of Uncertainty (3) is the one that fascinates statistical theorists. Sources (1) and (2) are the ones that obsess the rest of us. I looked at the first in Part 1 of this blog and, the second in Part 2. Now I want to look at the third Source of Uncertainty and try to put everything together.

If you are really most interested in (1) and (2), read “Prediction intervals” then skip forwards to “The fundamental theorem of forecasting”.

Prediction intervals

A prediction interval2 captures the range in which a future observation is expected to fall. Bafflingly, not all statistical software generates prediction intervals automatically so it is necessary, I fear, to know how to calculate them from first principles. However, understanding the calculation is, in itself, instructive.

But I emphasise that prediction intervals rely on a presumption that what is being forecast is “an environment that is sufficiently regular to be predictable”, that the (residual) business process data is exchangeable. If that presumption fails then all bets are off and we have to rely on a Cardinal Newman analysis. Of course, when I say that “all bets are off”, they aren’t. You will still be held to your existing contractual commitments even though your confidence in achieving them is now devastated. More on that another time.

Sources of variation in predictions

In the particular case of linear regression we need further to break down the third Source of Uncertainty.

  1. Uncertainty arising from the unexplained (residual) variation.
  2. Technical statistical sampling error in the regression calculation.
    1. Sampling error of the mean.
    2. Sampling error of the slope

Remember that we are, for the time being, assuming Source of Uncertainty (1) above can be disregarded. Let’s look at the other Sources of Uncertainty in turn: (2), (3A) and (3B).

Source of Variation (2) – Residual variation

We start with the Source of Uncertainty arising from the residual variation. This is the uncertainty because of all the things we don’t know. We talked about this a lot in Part 2. We are content, for the moment, that they are sufficiently stable to form a basis for prediction. We call this common cause variation. This variation has variance s2, where s is the residual standard deviation that will be output by your regression software.

RegressionResExpl2

Source of Variation (3A) – Sampling error in mean

To understand the next Source of Variation we need to know a little bit about how the regression is calculated. The calculations start off with the respective means of the X values ( X̄ ) and of the Y values ( Ȳ ). Uncertainty in estimating the mean of the Y , is the next contribution to the global prediction uncertainty.

An important part of calculating the regression line is to calculate the mean of the Ys. That mean is subject to sampling error. The variance of the sampling error is the familiar result from the statistics service course.

RegEq2

— where n is the number of pairs of X and Y. Obviously, as we collect more and more data this term gets more and more negligible.

RegressionMeanExpl

Source of Variation (3B) – Sampling error in slope

This is a bit more complicated. Skip forwards if you are already confused. Let me first give you the equation for the variance of predictions referable to sampling error in the slope.

RegEq3

This has now introduced the mysterious sum of squaresSXX. However, before we learn exactly what this is, we immediately notice two things.

  1. As we move away from the centre of the training data the variance gets larger.3
  2. As SXX gets larger the variance gets smaller.

The reason for the increasing sampling error as we move from the mean of X is obvious from thinking about how variation in slope works. The regression line pivots on the mean. Travelling further from the mean amplifies any disturbance in the slope.

RegressionSlopeExpl

Let’s look at where SXX comes from. The sum of squares is calculated from the Xs alone without considering the Ys. It is a characteristic of the sampling frame that we used to train the model. We take the difference of each X value from the mean of X, and then square that distance. To get the sum of squares we then add up all those individual squares. Note that this is a sum of the individual squares, not their average.

RegressionSXXTable

Two things then become obvious (if you think about it).

  1. As we get more and more data, SXX gets larger.
  2. As the individual Xs spread out over a greater range of XSXX gets larger.

What that (3B) term does emphasise is that even sampling error escalates as we exploit the edge of the original training data. As we extrapolate clear of the original sampling frame, the pure sampling error can quickly exceed even the residual variation.

Yet it is only a lower bound on the uncertainty in extrapolation. As we move away from the original range of Xs then, however happy we were previously with Source of Uncertainty (1), that the data was from “an environment that is sufficiently regular to be predictable”, then the question barges back in. We are now remote from our experience base in time and boundary. Nothing outside the original X-range will ever be a candidate for a comfort zone.

The fundamental theorem of prediction

Variances, generally, add up so we can sum the three Sources of Variation (2), (3A) and (3B). That gives the variance of an individual prediction, spred2. By an individual prediction I mean that somebody gives me an X and I use the regression formula to give them the (as yet unknown) corresponding Ypred.

RegEq4

It is immediately obvious that s2 is common to all three terms. However, the second and third terms, the sampling errors, can be made as small as we like by collecting more and more data. Collecting more and more data will have no impact on the first term. That arises from the residual variation. The stuff we don’t yet understand. It has variance s2, where s is the residual standard deviation that will be output by your regression software.

This, I say, is the fundamental theorem of prediction. The unexplained variation provides a hard limit on the precision of forecasts.

It is then a very simple step to convert the variance into a standard deviation, spred. This is the standard error of the prediction.4,5

RegEq5

Now, in general, where we have a measurement or prediction that has an uncertainty that can be characterised by a standard error u, there is an old trick for putting an interval round it. Remember that u is a measure of the variation in z. We can therefore put an interval around z as a number of standard errors, z±ku. Here, k is a constant of your choice. A prediction interval for the regression that generates prediction Ypred then becomes:

RegEq7

Choosing k=3 is very popular, conservative and robust.6,7 Other choices of k are available on the advice of a specialist mathematician.

It was Shewhart himself who took this all a bit further and defined tolerance intervals which contain a given proportion of future observations with a given probability.8 They are very much for the specialist.

Source of Variation (1) – Special causes

But all that assumes that we are sampling from “an environment that is sufficiently regular to be predictable”, that the residual variation is solely common cause. We checked that out on our original training data but the price of predictability is eternal vigilance. It can never be taken for granted. At any time fresh causes of variation may infiltrate the environment, or become newly salient because of some sensitising event or exotic interaction.

The real trouble with this world of ours is not that it is an unreasonable world, nor even that it is a reasonable one. The commonest kind of trouble is that it is nearly reasonable, but not quite. Life is not an illogicality; yet it is a trap for logicians. It looks just a little more mathematical and regular than it is; its exactitude is obvious, but its inexactitude is hidden; its wildness lies in wait.

G K Chesterton

The remedy for this risk is to continue plotting the residuals, the differences between the observed value and, now, the prediction. This is mandatory.

RegressionPBC2

Whenever we observe a signal of a potential special cause it puts us on notice to protect the forecast-user because our ability to predict the future has been exposed as deficient and fallible. But it also presents an opportunity. With timely investigation, a signal of a possible special cause may provide deeper insight into the variation of the cause-system. That in itself may lead to identifying further factors to build into the regression and a consequential reduction in s2.

It is reducing s2, by progressively accumulating understanding of the cause-system and developing the model, that leads to more precise, and more reliable, predictions.

Notes

  1. Kahneman, D (2011) Thinking, Fast and Slow, Allen Lane, p240
  2. Hahn, G J & Meeker, W Q (1991) Statistical Intervals: A Guide for Practitioners, Wiley, p31
  3. In fact s2/SXX is the sampling variance of the slope. The standard error of the slope is, notoriously, s/√SXX. A useful result sometimes. It is then obvious from the figure how variation is slope is amplified as we travel father from the centre of the Xs.
  4. Draper, N R & Smith, H (1998) Applied Regression Analysis, 3rd ed., Wiley, pp81-83
  5. Hahn & Meeker (1991) p232
  6. Wheeler, D J (2000) Normality and the Process Behaviour Chart, SPC Press, Chapter 6
  7. Vysochanskij, D F & Petunin, Y I (1980) “Justification of the 3σ rule for unimodal distributions”, Theory of Probability and Mathematical Statistics 21: 25–36
  8. Hahn & Meeker (1991) p231
Advertisement

The future of p-values

Another attack on p-values. This time by Regina Nuzzo in prestigious science journal Nature. Nuzzo advances the usual criticisms clearly and trenchantly. I hope that this will start to make people think hard about using probability to make decisions.

However, for me, the analysis still does not go deep enough. US baseball commentator Yogi Berra is reputed once to have observed that:

It’s tough to make predictions, especially about the future.

The fact that scientists work with confidence intervals, whereas if society is interested in such things it is interested in prediction intervals, belies the proper recognition of the future in much scientific writing.

The principal reason for doing statistics is to improve the reliability of predictions and forecasts. But the foundational question is whether the past is even representative of the future. Unless the past is representative of the future then it is of no assistance in forecasting. Many statisticians have emphasised the important property that any process must display to allow even tentative predictions about its future behaviour. Johnson and de Finetti called the property exchangeability, Shewhart and Deming called it statistical control, Don Wheeler coined the more suggestive term stable and predictable.

Shewhart once observed:

Both pure and applied science have gradually pushed further and further the requirements for accuracy and precision. However, applied science, particularly in the mass production of interchangeable parts, is even more exacting than pure science in certain matters of accuracy and precision.

Perhaps its unsurprising then that the concept is more widely relied upon in business than in scientific writing. All the same, statistical analysis begins and ends with considerations of stability. An analysis in which p-values do not assist.

At the head of this page is a tab labelled “Rearview” where I have surveyed the matter more widely. I would like to think of this as supplementary to Nuzzo’s views.